From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Salary窶田elery merger)

In the history of English phonology, there have been many diachronic sound changes affecting vowels, especially involving phonemic splits and mergers. A number of these changes are specific to vowels which occur before /l/, especially in cases where the /l/ is at the end of a syllable (or is not followed by a vowel).

Historical diphthongization before /l/

Diphthongization occurred since Early Modern English in certain -al- and -ol- sequences before coronal or velar consonants, or at the end of a word or morpheme. In these sequences, /al/ became /awl/ and then /ノ爽l/, while /ノ罵/ became /ノ背l/ and then /ノ盃l/. Both of these merged with existing diphthongs: /ノ爽/ as in law and /ノ盃/ as in throw.

At the end of a word or morpheme, this produced /ノ爽l/in all, ball, call, fall, gall, hall, mall, small, squall, stall, pall, tall, thrall and wall; /ノ盃l/ in control, droll, extol, knoll, poll (meaning a survey of people,) roll, scroll, stroll, swollen, toll, and troll. The word shall did not follow this trend, and remains /ハεヲl/ today.

Before coronal consonants, this produced /ノ爽l/ in Alderney, alter, bald, balderdash, false, falter, halt, malt, palsy, salt, Wald and Walter; /ノ盃l/ in bold, cold, fold, gold, hold, molten, mould/mold, old, shoulder (earlier sholder), smolder, told, and wold (in the sense of "tract of land"). As with shall, the word shalt did not follow this trend, and remains /ハεヲlt/ today.

Before /k/, this produced /ノ爽l/ in balk, caulk/calk, chalk, Dundalk, falcon, stalk, talk and walk; /ノ盃l/ in folk, Polk, and yolk.

This L-vocalization established a pattern that would influence the spelling pronunciations of some relatively more recent loanwords like Balt, Malta, waltz, Yalta, and polder. It also influenced English spelling reform efforts, explaining the American English mold and molt vs. the traditional mould and moult.

Certain words of more recent origin or coining, however, do not have the change and retain short vowels, including Al, alcohol, bal, Cal, calcium, gal, Hal, mal-, pal, Sal, talc, Val, doll, Moll, and Poll (a nickname for a parrot.)

The Great Vowel Shift altered the pronunciation of the diphthongs, with /ノ爽/ becoming the monophthong /ノ藩/, and /ノ盃/ raising to /oハ/.

Historical L-vocalization

In -alk and -olk words, the /l/ subsequently disappeared entirely in most accents (with the notable exception of Hiberno-English). This change caused /ノ爽lk/ to become /ノ爽k/, and /ノ盃lk/ to become /ノ盃k/. Even outside Ireland, some of these words have more than one pronunciation that retains the /l/ sound, especially in American English where spelling pronunciations caused partial or full reversal of L-vocalization in a handful of cases:

  • caulk/calk can be /ヒkノ藩人k/ or /ヒkノ藩震/.
  • falcon can be /ヒfテヲlkノ冢/, /ヒfノ藩人kノ冢/ or /ヒfノ藩震ノ冢/.
  • yolk can be /ヒjoハ獲k/ or /ヒjoハ殻/. yoke as /ヒjoハ殻/ is only conditionally homophonous.

Words like fault and vault did not undergo L-vocalization, but rather L-restoration, having previously been L-vocalized independently in Old French and lacking the /l/ in Middle English, but having it restored by Early Modern English. The word falcon existed simultaneously as homonyms fauco(u)n and falcon in Middle English. The word moult/molt never originally had /l/ to begin with, instead deriving from Middle English mout and related etymologically to mutate; the /l/ joined the word intrusively.

The loss of /l/ in words spelt with -alf, -alm, -alve and -olm did not involve L-vocalization in the same sense, but rather the elision of the consonant and usually the compensatory lengthening of the vowel.

Variation between /ノ藩人/ and /ノ値/ before a consonant in salt and similar words

Some words such as salt, traditionally pronounced by most RP speakers with /ノ藩人/ followed by a consonant, have alternative pronunciations with /ノ値/ that are used more frequently by younger British English speakers. This variation between /ノ藩人/ and /ノ値/ occurs primarily before voiceless consonants, as in salt, false and alter; less commonly, /ノ値/ may also be used in words where the /l/ comes before a voiced consonant, as in bald, scald and cauldron. [1] [2] In Great Britain, this laxing before /l/ was traditionally associated with Northern England and Wales, [3] but has in recent decades become more widespread, including among younger speakers of RP. [2]

Modern L-vocalization

More extensive L-vocalization is a notable feature of certain dialects of English, including Cockney, Estuary English, New York English, New Zealand English, Pittsburgh and Philadelphia English, in which an /l/ sound occurring at the end of a word or before a consonant is pronounced as some sort of close back vocoid, e.g., [w], [o] or [ハ馨. The resulting sound may not always be rounded. The precise phonetic quality varies. It can be heard occasionally in the dialect of the English East Midlands, where words ending in -old can be pronounced /oハ嚇/. KM Petyt (1985) noted this feature in the traditional dialect of West Yorkshire but said it has died out. [4] However, in recent decades l-vocalization has been spreading outwards from London and the south east, [5] [6] John C Wells argued that it is probable that it will become the standard pronunciation in England over the next one hundred years, [7] an idea which Petyt criticised in a book review. [8]

In Cockney, Estuary English and New Zealand English, l-vocalization can be accompanied by phonemic mergers of vowels before the vocalized /l/, so that real, reel and rill, which are distinct in most dialects of English, are homophones as [ノケノェw].

Graham Shorrocks noted extensive L-vocalisation in the dialect of Bolton, Greater Manchester and commented, "many, perhaps, associate such a quality more with Southern dialects, than with Lancashire/Greater Manchester." [9]

In the accent of Bristol, syllabic /l/ can be vocalized to /o/, resulting in pronunciations like /ヒbノ稚o/ (for bottle). By hypercorrection, however, some words originally ending in /o/ were given an /l/: the original name of the town was Bristow, but this has been altered by hypercorrection to Bristol. [10]

African-American English (AAE) dialects may have L-vocalization as well. However, in these dialects, it may be omitted altogether (e.g. fool becomes [fuヒ疹. Some English speakers from San Francisco - particularly those of Asian ancestry - also vocalize or omit /l/. [11]

Salary窶田elery merger

The salary窶田elery merger is a conditioned merger of /テヲ/ (as in bat) and /ノ/ (as in bet) when they occur before /l/, thus making salary and celery homophones. [12] [13] [14] [15] The merger is not well studied. It is referred to in various sociolinguistic publications, but usually only as a small section of the larger change undergone by vowels preceding /l/ in articles about l-vocalization.

This merger has been detected in the English spoken in New Zealand and in parts of the Australian state of Victoria, including the capital Melbourne. [16] [17] The merger is also found in the Norfuk dialect spoken on Norfolk Island. [15] The salary-celery merger is also characteristic of Chicano English in Los Angeles and has been attested in the Chicano English of northern New Mexico and Albuquerque as well. [18] [19] [20] /ノ/ is also often lowered before /l/ in El Paso, but not all speakers show a merger. [21] In varieties with the merger, salary and celery are both pronounced /sテヲlノ决i/. [13]

The study presented by Cox and Palethorpe at a 2003 conference tested just one group of speakers from Victoria: 13 fifteen-year-old girls from a Catholic girls' school in Wangaratta. Their pronunciations were compared with those of school girl groups in the towns of Temora, Junee and Wagga Wagga in New South Wales. In the study conducted by Cox and Palethorpe, the group in Wangaratta exhibited the merger while speakers in Temora, Junee and Wagga Wagga did not. [13]

Deborah Loakes from Melbourne University has suggested that the salary-celery merger is restricted to Melbourne and southern Victoria, not being found in northern border towns such as Albury-Wodonga or Mildura. [16]

In the 2003 study Cox and Palethorpe note that the merger appears to only involve lowering of /e/ before /l/, with the reverse not occurring, stating that "There is no evidence in this data of raised /テヲ/ before /l/ as in 'Elbert' for 'Albert', a phenomenon that has been popularly suggested for Victorians." [13]

Horsfield (2001) investigates the effects of postvocalic /l/ on the preceding vowels in New Zealand English; her investigation covers all of the New Zealand English vowels and is not specifically tailored to studying mergers and neutralizations, but rather the broader change that occurs across the vowels. She has suggested that further research involving minimal pairs like telly and tally, celery and salary should be done before any firm conclusions are drawn.

A pilot study of the merger was done, which yielded perception and production data from a few New Zealand speakers. The results of the pilot survey suggested that although the merger was not found in the speech of all participants, those who produced a distinction between /テヲl/ and /el/ also accurately perceived a difference between them; those who merged /テヲl/ and /el/ were less able to accurately perceive the distinction. The finding has been interesting to some linguists because it concurs with the recent understanding that losing a distinction between two sounds involves losing the ability to produce it as well as to perceive it (Gordon 2002). However, due to the very small number of people participating in the study the results are not conclusive.

Homophonous pairs
/テヲl/ /ノ嬪/ IPA Notes
Allan Ellen テヲlノ冢
bally belly bテヲli
dally Delhi dテヲli
dally deli dテヲli
fallow fellow fテヲloハ
Hal hell hテヲl
mallow mellow mテヲloハ
Sal cel sテヲl
Sal cell sテヲl
Sal sell sテヲl
salary celery sテヲlノ决i
shall shell ハεヲl

Fill窶吐eel merger

The areas marked in red are where the fill窶吐eel merger is most consistently present in the local accent. Map based on Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006: 71). [22]

The fill窶吐eel merger is a conditioned merger of the vowels /ノェ/ and /iヒ/ before /l/ that occurs in some accents. In Europe, it is commonly found in Estuary English. Otherwise it is typical of certain accents of American English. The heaviest concentration of the merger is found in, but not necessarily confined to, Southern American English: in North Carolina, eastern Tennessee, northern Alabama, Mississippi, northern and central Louisiana (but not New Orleans), and west-central Texas (Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 69-73). This merger, like many other features of Southern American English, can also be found in AAE.

Homophonous pairs
/ノェl/ /iヒ人/ IPA Notes
dill deal dノェl
fill feel fノェl
filled field fノェld
hill heal hノェl
hill heel hノェl
hill he'll hノェl
ill eel ノェl
Jill geal dハ痛ェl
kill keel kノェl
lil leal lノェl
lil Lille mノェl
mill meal mノェl
nil kneel nノェl
nil Neil nノェl
Phil feel fノェl
pill peal pノェl
pill peel pノェl
rill real rノェl
rill reel rノェl
shill she'll ハλェl
shilled shield ハλェld
sill ceil sノェl
sill seal sノェl
silly Seely sノェli
spill spiel spノェl When spiel is not pronounced with initial /ハ-/
still steal stノェl
still steel stノェl
till teal tノェl
will we'll wノェl
will wheel wノェl With wine-whine merger.
willed wield wノェld

Fell窶吐ail merger

The same two regions show a closely related merger, namely the fell窶吐ail merger of /ノ/ and /eノェ/ before /l/ that occurs in some varieties of Southern American English making fell and fail homophones. In addition to North Carolina and Texas, these mergers are found sporadically in other Southern states and in the Midwest and West. [23] [24]

Homophonous pairs
/ノ嬪/ /eノェl/ IPA Notes
bell bail bノ嬪
bell bale bノ嬪
belle bail bノ嬪
belle bale bノ嬪
cell, cel sail sノ嬪
cell, cel sale sノ嬪
dell dale dノ嬪
ell ail ノ嬪
ell ale ノ嬪
fell fail fノ嬪
gel gaol, jail dハ痛嬪
geld galed gノ嬪d
held hailed hノ嬪d
hell hail hノ嬪
hell hale hノ嬪
knell nail nノ嬪
L, ell ail ノ嬪
L, ell ale ノ嬪
Mel mail mノ嬪
Mel male mノ嬪
meld mailed mノ嬪d
Nell nail nノ嬪
quell quail kwノ嬪
sell sail sノ嬪
sell sale sノ嬪
shell shale ハλ嬪
swell swale swノ嬪
tell tail tノ嬪
tell tale tノ嬪
weld wailed wノ嬪d
well wail wノ嬪
well wale wノ嬪
wells wales wノ嬪z
wells Wales wノ嬪z
well whale wノ嬪 With wine-whine merger.
wells wails wノ嬪z
wells whales wノ嬪z With wine-whine merger.
yell Yale jノ嬪

Full窶吐ool merger

The full窶吐ool merger is a conditioned merger of /ハ/ and /uヒ/ before /l/, making pairs like pull/pool and full/fool homophones. The main concentration of the pull窶菟ool merger is in Western Pennsylvania English, centered around Pittsburgh. The merger is less consistently but still noticeably present in some speakers of surrounding Midland American English. [25] The Atlas of North American English also reports this merger, or near-merger, scattered sporadically throughout Western American English, with particular prevalence in some speakers of urban Utahn, Californian, and New Mexican English. [26] Accents with L-vocalization, such as New Zealand English, Estuary English and Cockney, may also have the full窶吐ool merger in most cases, but when a suffix beginning with a vowel is appended, the distinction returns: Hence 'pull' and 'pool' are [pハ覚], but 'pulling' is /ヒpハ獲ノェナ/ whereas 'pooling' remains /ヒpuヒ人ノェナ/. [27]

The fill窶吐eel merger and full窶吐ool merger are not unified in American English; they are found in different parts of the country, and very few people show both mergers. [28]

Homophonous pairs
/ハ獲/ /uヒ人/ IPA (using ⟨uヒ⟩ for the merged vowel)
bull boule buヒ人
full fool fuヒ人
pull pool puヒ人

Hull窶塗ole merger

The hull窶塗ole merger is a conditioned merger of /ハ/ and /oハ/ before /l/ occurring for some speakers of English English with l-vocalization. As a result, "hull" and "hole" are homophones as [hノ般馨. The merger is also mentioned by Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006: 72) as a merger before /l/ in North American English that might require further study. The latter merger can also involve /ハ/ or /ノ/ before /l/.

Homophonous pairs
/ハ畦/ /oハ獲/ /ハ獲/ /ノ冤/ IPA Notes
adult a dolt ノ厖dVlt Adult as /dlt/.
bold bulled bVld
bowl bull bVl
bowled bulled bVld
culled cold kVld
cull coal kVl
cull cole kVl
cult colt kVlt
dull dole dVl
foal full fVl
foaled fulled fVld
fold fulled fVld
gull goal ノ。Vl
hull hole hVl
hull whole hVl
hulled hold hVld
hulled holed hVld
mull mole mVl
mulled mold mVld
mulled mould mVld
null gnoll nVl
null knoll nVl
pole pull pVl
poll pull pVl
Seminole seminal ヒsノ嬶ノェnVl
skulled scold skVld
sull sole sVl
sull soul sVl
sulled sold sVld
sulled soled sVld
sulled souled sVld

Gulf-golf merger

The gulf-golf merger is the merger of the diaphonemes /ハ/ and /ノ/ before /lC/, where C denotes a consonant. It is attested in Australian English, in which it can co-occur with the Doll-dole merger. In Australian English the result of this 2-3 way merger is [ノ脳, the vowel of LOT. [29]

Homophonous pairs
/ハ畦/ /ノ値/ IPA (using ⟨⟩ for the merged vowel) Notes
cult colt kノ値t With the doll-dole merger
exult exalt ノェgヒzノ値t
gulf golf gノ値f

Doll窶電ole merger

The doll窶電ole merger is a conditioned merger for many Southern England English, Australian English [30] and New Zealand English [31] speakers, of /ノ/ and /ノ厂/ before syllable-final (or non-prevocalic) /l/, resulting in homophony between pairs like doll and dole. [32] The distinction between /ノ/ and /ノ厂/ is maintained in derived forms containing prevocalic /l/, such as d[ノ綻lling herself up vs. d[ノ通馨ling it out, which means that the underlying vowel is recoverable if the /l/ is morpheme-final, as in doll and dole. [32] But when the /l/ is followed by a consonant within the same morpheme, as in solve, the distinction is not recoverable; this may be the cause, via hypercorrection, of pronunciations such as [sノ厂獲v] for solve in place of RP [sノ値v]. [32]

Homophonous pairs
/ノ値/ /oハ獲/ IPA (using ⟨⟩ for the merged vowel) Notes
Balt bolt bノ値t When Balt is not pronounced as /bノ藩人t/ [a]
doll dole dノ値
halt holt mノ値t When halt is not pronounced as /hノ藩人t/ [a]
malt moult mノ値t When malt is not pronounced as /mノ藩人t/ [a]
moll mole mノ値
paltry poultry pノ値tri When paltry is not pronounced as /pノ藩人tri/ [a]
poll pole pノ値 Already homophonous in dialects that pronounce poll as /poハ獲/ [b]
vol vole vノ値
vault volt vノ値t When vault is not pronounced as /vノ藩人t/ [a]

Goat split

The goat split is a process that has affected London dialects, Australian English, and Estuary English. [35] [36] In the first phase of the split, the diphthong of goat /ノ厂/ developed an allophone [ノ通馨 before "dark" (nonprevocalic) /l/. Thus goal no longer had the same vowel as goat ([ノ。ノ通緩ォ] vs. [ノ。ノ厂缶脳). [35] In the second phase, the diphthong [ノ通馨 spread to other forms of affected words. For example, the realization of rolling changed from [ヒ伊ケノ厂獲ノェナ犠 to [ヒ伊ケノ通獲ノェナ犠 on the model of roll [ノケノ通緩ォ]. This led to the creation of a minimal pair for some speakers: wholly /ヒhノ通獲i/ vs. holy /ヒhノ厂獲i/ and thus to phonemicization of the split. The change from /ノ厂/ to /ノ通/ in derived forms is not fully consistent; for instance, in cockney, polar is pronounced with the /ノ厂/ of goat even though it is derived from pole /ヒpノ通獲/.

In broad Cockney, the phonetic difference between the two phonemes may be rather small and they may be distinguished by nothing more than the openness of the first element, so that goat is pronounced [ノ。ノ惜、ハ脳 whereas goal is pronounced [ノ。aノ、]. [35]

Goose split

Similar to the Goat split, the Goose vowel has developed contrasting phonetic outcomes before /l/ in some Southeastern English dialects, exhibited by the pair ruler (measuring instrument), pronounced with a fronter vowel that can be transcribed [yヒ疹 [37] or [ハ駅], [38] and ruler ('one who rules'), pronounced with a backer vowel that can be transcribed [uヒ疹, [37] [ハ学] or [oヒ疹. [38] This contrast developed from an allophonic distribution where a back variant of the goose vowel is used before tautosyllabic /l/, as in rule /ヒruヒ人/ [ヒ伊ケuヒ惜ォ], but a fronted variant closer to [yヒ疹 is used elsewhere, as in ruler (instrument) /ヒruヒ.lノ/ [ヒ伊ケyヒ.lノ兢.

This distribution has become complicated by morphology in a way that is leading to a phonemic split in words with pre-vocalic /l/: those where the /l/ is stem-final are pronounced with the phonetically back vowel [uヒ疹 (as in ruler (monarch), a morphologically transparent derivative of rule), whereas those where the /l/ is stem-medial are pronounced with a fronted vowel [yヒ疹 (as in ruler (measuring instrument), which is treated as an unanalyzable unit). The difference in vowel quality is presumably accompanied by a difference in the pronunciation of the following /l/ [37] ([ノォ] after [uヒ疹, [l] after [yヒ疹).

A similar backing change has occurred in many North American dialects, [39] but this has remained allophonic. For example, in California English, the Goose vowel is realized as a back vowel in words such as school where it is followed by /l/, but is fronted in words where it is not followed by /l/, such as new. [40]

Fool窶吐all merger

For some English speakers in the UK, the vowels of goose and thought may be merged before dark syllable-final /l/, which may be caused by the raising of the thought vowel to [oヒ疹 or [ハ翰疹 in combination with the backing of the goose vowel before /l/ as part of the Goose split. [41] This neutralization has been found to exist for clusters of speakers in the southern UK, especially for speakers from areas of the south coast and the Greater London area. [42]

Homophonous pairs
/uヒ人/ /ノ藩人/ IPA (using ⟨oヒ⟩ for the merged vowel)
boule ball boヒ人
boule bawl boヒ人
cool call koヒ人
cruel crawl koヒ人
drool drawl droヒ人
fool fall foヒ人
ghoul gall goヒ人
ghoul Gaul goヒ人
pool pall poヒ人
pool Paul poヒ人
schooled scald skoヒ人d
stool stall stoヒ人
tool tall toヒ人
Yule yawl joヒ人

Vile窶砺ial merger

The vile窶砺ial merger is where the words in the vile set ending with /-ヒaノェl/ (bile, file, guile, I'll, Kyle, Lyle, mile, Nile, pile, rile, smile, stile, style, tile, vile, while, wile) rhyme with words in the vial set ending with /-ヒaノェノ冤/ (decrial, denial, dial, espial, Niall, phial, trial, vial, viol). [43] This merger involves the dephonemicization of schwa that occurs after a vowel and before /l/, causing the vowel-/l/ sequence to be pronounced as either one or two syllables.

This merger may also be encountered with other vowel rhymes too, including:

  • /-ヒeノェl/ (jail, sale, tail, etc.) and /-ヒeノェノ冤/ (betrayal, Jael), usually skewing towards two syllables.
  • /-ヒ伊繁ェl/ (coil, soil, etc.) and /-ヒ伊繁ェノ冤/ (loyal, royal), usually skewing towards two syllables.
  • /-ヒiヒ人/ (ceil, feel, steal, etc.) and /-ヒiヒ惜冤/ (real), usually skewing towards two syllables.
  • /-ヒ伊藩人/ (all, drawl, haul, etc.) and /-ヒ伊藩惜冤/ (withdrawal), usually skewing towards one syllable.
  • /-ヒoハ獲/ (bowl, coal, hole, roll, soul, etc.) and /-ヒoハ緩冤/ (Joel, Noel), usually skewing towards one syllable.
  • /-ヒuヒ人/ (cool, ghoul, mewl, rule, you'll, etc.) and /-ヒuヒ惜冤/ (cruel, dual, duel, fuel, gruel, jewel), usually skewing towards one syllable.
  • /-ヒaハ獲/ (owl, scowl, etc.) and /-ヒaハ緩冤/ (bowel, dowel, Powell, towel, trowel, vowel), inconsistently skewing towards either one or two syllables. Some words may wander across this boundary even in some non-merging accents, such as owl with /-ヒaハ緩冤/, and bowel with /-ヒaハ獲/.
  • In some rhotic accents, /-ヒ伊徨l/ (girl, hurl, pearl, etc.) and /-ヒ伊徨ノ冤/ (referral), usually skewing towards two syllables. This historically happened to the word squirrel, which was previously /ヒskwノェrノ冤/ (and still is in certain accents) but became one syllable /ヒskwノ徨l/ in General American today. Some accents with one-syllable squirrel later broke it into two syllables again, as /ヒskwノ徨ノ冤/.
  • In some rhotic father窶澱other merged accents, /-ヒ伊喪l/ (Carl, marl, etc.) and /-ヒ伊喪ノ冤/ (coral, moral), usually skewing towards two syllables.

For many speakers, the vowels in cake, meet, vote and moot can become centering diphthongs before /l/, leading to pronunciations like [teノ冤], [tiノ冤], [toノ冤] and [tuノ冤] for tail, teal, toll and tool.

Merger of non-prevocalic /ハ獲/, /uヒ人/, /ノ冤/, /ノ藩人/ with morpheme-internal /ノ藩/

Cockney features a THOUGHT-split whereby the /ノ藩/ (the THOUGHT-NORTH-FORCE vowel) is pronounced differently depending on its position in the syllable structure: [oヒ疹 in checked syllables and [ノ繁兢 in free syllables.

The L-vocalization of Cockney can leads to non-prevocalic /l/ being pronounced with a quality around [o], resulting in it being entirely absorbed by the preceding [oヒ疹 when it folllows a (by definition, morpheme-internal free syllable) THOUGHT vowel in words such as bald, call and Paul, leading to homophonous pairs such as bald and board ([boヒ薪]), called and cord ([koヒ薪]), Paul's and pause ([poヒ諏]). [44]

Such homophones can only arise when the word without a historic /l/ also has the THOUGHT-NORTH-FORCE vowel in a morpheme-internal position, as in morpheme-final positions it will be pronounced as [ノ繁兢 rather than [oヒ疹, thus Paul's ([poヒ諏]) and paws ([pノ繁凛]), bald ([boヒ薪]) and bored ([bノ繁囘]) etc remain distinct.

The full-fool and fool-fall mergers, both of which are common in Cockney, can cause /ハ獲/, /uヒ人/ to also merge with morpheme-internal /ノ藩/, leading to homophonous pairs such as wolf and wharf [woヒ診] and cools and cause [koヒ諏]; and 'pulls, pools, Paul's and pause all becoming homophonous as [poヒ諏]. [45]

Non-prevocalic/ノ冤/ (as in bottle) can also merge with morpheme-interal /ノ藩/, leading to musical being homophonous with music hall as [ヒmjuヒ諏ノェkoヒ疹. Cockney speakers usually regard both syllables of awful as rhyming: [ヒoヒ診oヒ疹. [44]

In the following list, the only homophonous pairs that are included are those involving a word with /l/ and a word without. As the merger is restricted to non-rhotic accents, morpheme-internal /ノ藩/ in the fifth columns is assumed to cover not only THOUGHT but also NORTH and FORCE.

Potentially homophonous pairs
/ハ獲/ /uヒ人/ /ノ藩人/ Morpheme-internal /ノ藩/ IPA (using ⟨oヒ⟩ for the merged vowel) Notes
Alt ought ヒoヒ腎 When alt is not pronounced /ヒ伊値t/
assault assort ヒoヒ腎 When assault is not pronounced /ノ厖sノ値t/
Balt bought ヒboヒ腎 When Balt is not pronounced /ヒbノ値t/
bulled bald board ヒboヒ薪
baldy bawdy ヒboヒ薪i
bulled balled board ヒboヒ薪
bulled bawled board ヒboヒ薪
brawled broad ヒbroヒ薪
coolled called cord ヒkoヒ薪
cools calls cause ヒkoヒ諏
false force ヒfoヒ尽 When false is not pronounced /ヒfノ値s/
fault fort ヒfoヒ腎 When fault is not pronounced /ヒfノ値t/
fault fought ヒfoヒ腎 When fault is not pronounced /ヒfノ値t/
fault thought ヒfoヒ腎 With th-fronting, when fault is not pronounced /ヒfノ値t/.
fulled fooled ford ヒfoヒ薪
galled gourd ヒgoヒ薪 With cure-force merger
halls Hawes ヒhoヒ諏
hauls Hawes ヒhoヒ諏
hauled hoard ヒhoヒ薪
Malden Morden ヒmoヒ薪ノ冢
malt mort ヒmoヒ腎 When malt is not pronounced /ヒmノ値t/
Malta mortar ヒmoヒ腎ノ When malt is not pronounced /ヒmノ値tノ/
mauled Maud ヒmoヒ腎 When malt is not pronounced /ヒmノ値t/
pulls pools Paul's pause ヒpoヒ諏
salt sort ヒsoヒ腎 When salt is not pronounced /ヒsノ値t/
salt sought ヒsoヒ腎 When salt is not pronounced /ヒsノ値t/
wolf wharf ヒwoヒ診
Walt wart ヒwoヒ腎 When Walt is not pronounced /ヒwノ値t/
Walter water ヒwoヒ腎ノ When Walter is not pronounced /ヒwノ値tノ/

Other mergers

Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006:73) mention four mergers before /l/ that may be under way in some accents of North American English, and which require more study: [46]

  • /ハ獲/ and /oハ獲/ (bull vs bowl)
  • /ハ畦/ and /ノ藩人/ (hull vs hall)
  • /ハ獲/ and /ハ畦/ (bull vs hull) (effectively undoing the foot-strut split before /l/)
  • /ハ畦/ and /oハ獲/ (hull vs bowl)

See also

Notes

  1. ^ a b c d e Words like Balt, halt, malt, paltry and vault can be pronounced with /ノ値t/ or /ノ藩人t/ in British English, but the /ノ値t/ pronunciation is used by the majority of younger speakers, see #Variation between /ノ藩人/ and /ノ値/ before a consonant in salt and similar words.
  2. ^ Poll is variably pronounced as /pノ値/ and /poハ獲/ in British English, while pole is always pronounced /poハ獲/ by speakers without the merger. [33] [34]

References

  1. ^ Wells, John (2010). " scolding water" (February 16). John Wells窶冱 phonetic blog. Retrieved 2016-01-31.
  2. ^ a b Lindsey, Geoff (2019). English After RP: Standard British Pronunciation Today. p. 39-41, 125. ISBN  9783030043568.
  3. ^ Wells, John C. (June 1999). "British English pronunciation preferences: a changing scene". Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 29: 36. Retrieved 2023-09-01.
  4. ^ KM Petyt, Dialect & Accent in Industrial West Yorkshire, John Benjamins Publishing Company, page 219
  5. ^ Asher, R.E., Simpson, J.M.Y. (1993). The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics. Pergamon. p. 4043. ISBN  978-0080359434
  6. ^ Kortmann, Bernd et al. (2004). A Handbook of Varieties of English. Mouton de Gruyter. p. 196. ISBN  978-3110175325.
  7. ^ Wells (1982), p. 259.
  8. ^ Petyt, KM (1982). "Reviews: JC Wells: Accents of English". Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 12 (2). Cambridge: 104窶112. doi: 10.1017/S0025100300002516. S2CID  146349564. Retrieved 6 January 2013.
  9. ^ Shorrocks, Graham (1999). A Grammar of the Dialect of the Bolton Area. Pt. 2: Morphology and syntax. Bamberger Beitrテ、ge zur englischen Sprachwissenschaft; Bd. 42. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. p. 255. ISBN  3-631-34661-1. (based on the author's thesis (Ph. D.)--University of Sheffield, 1981)
  10. ^ Harper, Douglas. "Bristol". Online Etymology Dictionary.
  11. ^ L Hall-Lew & RL Starr, Beyond the 2nd generation: English use among Chinese Americans in the San Francisco Bay Area, English Today: The International Review of the English Language, Vol. 26, Issue 3, pp. 12-19. [1]
  12. ^ Cox, F.; Palethorpe, S. (2001). "The Changing Face of Australian Vowels". In Blair, D.B.; Collins, P (eds.). Varieties of English Around the World: English in Australia. John Benjamins Publishing, Amsterdam. pp. 17窶44.
  13. ^ a b c d Cox, F. M.; Palethorpe, S. (2004). "The border effect: Vowel differences across the NSW窶天ictorian Border". Proceedings of the 2003 Conference of the Australian Linguistic Society: 1窶14.
  14. ^ Palethorpe, Sallyanne; Cox, Felicity (2003). Vowel Modification in Pre-lateral Environments (PDF). International Seminars on Speech Production. ISBN  1-86408-871-0. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-03-07.
  15. ^ a b Ingram, John. Norfolk Island-Pitcairn English (Pitkern Norfolk) Archived 2009-02-25 at the Wayback Machine, University of Queensland, 2006
  16. ^ a b Are Melburnians mangling the language?
  17. ^ The /el/-/テヲl/ Sound Change in Australian English: A Preliminary Perception Experiment, Deborah Loakes, John Hajek and Janet Fletcher, University of Melbourne
  18. ^ Penfield, Joyce (1985). Chicano English: an ethnic contact dialect. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Pub. Co. p. 45. ISBN  9789027248657.
  19. ^ Hernテ。ndez, Pilar (1993). "Vowel shift in Northern New Mexico Chicano English". Mester. 22 (2): 227窶234. doi: 10.5070/M3222014266.
  20. ^ Brumbaugh, Susan (2017). Anglo and Hispanic Vowel Variation in New Mexican English (PhD). University of New Mexico. Retrieved March 15, 2022.
  21. ^ Williams, Lance Levi (2010). /モ/ and /e/ in El Paso English (MA). University of Texas at El Paso.
  22. ^ "Map 4". Ling.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2011-03-02.
  23. ^ "Map 7". Ling.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2011-03-02.
  24. ^ "Chapter 11". www.ling.upenn.edu. Archived from the original on 28 October 2006. Retrieved 12 January 2022.
  25. ^ "Map 5". Ling.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2011-03-02.
  26. ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006:70)
  27. ^ "Transcribing Estuary English". Phon.ucl.ac.uk. Retrieved 2011-03-02.
  28. ^ "Map 6". Ling.upenn.edu. Retrieved 2011-03-02.
  29. ^ Lewis, Eleanor. "/ノ人C/-/ノ罵C/ Sound change in Australian English: Preliminary res[ノ]lts". {{ cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= ( help)
  30. ^ Burridge, Kate (2004). Bernd Kortmann and Edgar W. Schneider (ed.). A Handbook of Varieties of English Volume 1: Phonology. De Gruyter. p. 1090.
  31. ^ Bauer, Laurie; Warren, Paul (2004). Bernd Kortmann and Edgar W. Schneider (ed.). A Handbook of Varieties of English Volume 1: Phonology. De Gruyter. p. 589.
  32. ^ a b c Wells (1982), p. 317
  33. ^ "POLL | English meaning". Cambridge Dictionary. Retrieved 2023-09-03.
  34. ^ "POLE | English meaning". Cambridge Dictionary. Retrieved 2023-09-03.
  35. ^ a b c Wells (1982), pp. 312窶313
  36. ^ Altendorf, Ulrike (2003). Estuary English: Levelling at the Interface of RP and South-Eastern British English. Tテシbingen: Gunter Narr Verlag. p. 34. ISBN  3-8233-6022-1.
  37. ^ a b c Wells, John (3 February 2012). "newly minimal". John Wells窶冱 phonetic blog. Retrieved 19 April 2023.
  38. ^ a b Lindsey, Geoff (24 December 2013). "GOOSE backing". Speech Talk blog. Retrieved 19 April 2023.
  39. ^ William Labov: The Changing Patterns of Philadelphia English, retrieved 2022-09-26
  40. ^ Eckert, Penelope. "Vowel Shifts in California and the Detroit Suburbs". Retrieved July 5, 2023.
  41. ^ Lindsey, Geoff (11 September 2016). "People fool in love (extended mix)". Speech Talk Blog. Retrieved 25 March 2018.
  42. ^ MacKenzie, Laurel; Bailey, George; Turton, Danielle (2016). "Who pronounces 'fool' and 'fall' the same?". Our Dialects: Mapping variation in English in the UK. Retrieved 25 March 2018.
  43. ^ According to Dictionary.com, dial, trial and vial all specify variable /-ヒaノェノ冤/ or /-ヒaノェl/ pronunciations, while words like bile and style only specify /-ヒaノェl/ pronunciations.
  44. ^ a b Wells (1982), p. 314.
  45. ^ Wells (1982), p. 316.
  46. ^ Labov, William; Sharon Ash; Charles Boberg (2006). The Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton-de Gruyter. ISBN  3-11-016746-8.

Bibliography

  • Gordon, Elizabeth; Maclagan, Margaret (2004), "Regional and social differences in New Zealand: phonology", in Schneider, Edgar W.; Burridge, Kate; Kortmann, Bernd; Mesthrie, Rajend; Upton, Clive (eds.), A handbook of varieties of English, vol. 1: Phonology, Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 603窶613, doi: 10.1515/9783110197181-039, ISBN  3-11-017532-0
  • Horsfield, Rachel (2001). The Changing Vowels of New Zealand English (Thesis). University of Otago.
  • Labov, William; Ash, Sharon; Boberg, Charles (2006). The Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN  978-3-11-016746-7.
  • Wells, John C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN  9780521246484.